Defining whole language
By Dr. Samuel L. Blumenfeld
The other day, I received an e-mail from a lady in California who asked, "What
on earth is the whole-language system?" She had read my article on the making of
the black underclass in which I had identified whole language as the primary cause
of reading failure among so many black students.
Fortunately, the answer is easy to give, because whole-language professors have
been quite open in defining what they mean by their pedagogic philosophy. So I shall
quote some salient passages from their writings.
In a book entitled "Whole Language, What's the Difference?" written by three
whole-language professors in 1991, we read on page 32:
Whole language represents a major shift in thinking about the reading process.
Rather than viewing reading as "getting the words," whole language educators
view reading as essentially a process of creating meanings ... Meaning is created
through a transaction with whole, meaningful texts (i.e., texts of any
length that were written with the intent to communicate meaning).
It is a transaction, not an extraction of the meaning from the print,
in the sense that the reader-created meanings are a fusion of what the
reader brings and what the text offers ... Although students who learn to read
in whole language classrooms are, like all proficient readers, eventually able
to "read" (or identify) a large inventory of words, learning words is certainly
not the goal of whole language.
Another passage from page 19 of the same book may be even more illuminating:
From a whole-language perspective, reading (and language use in general)
is a process of generating hypotheses in a meaning-making transaction in a sociohistorical
context. As a transactional process ... reading is not a matter of "getting
the meaning" from text, as if that meaning were in the text waiting to
be decoded by the reader.
Rather, reading is a matter of readers using the cues print provides and
the knowledge they bring with them (of language subsystems, of the world) to
construct a unique interpretation.
Moreover, that interpretation is situated: readers' creations (not
retrievals) of meaning with text vary, depending on their purposes of reading
and the expectations of others in the reading event. This view of reading implies
that there is no single "correct" meaning for a given text, only plausible meanings.
Now you might think that all of this pedagogical insanity is taking place in
some kind of political vacuum. Nothing could be farther from the truth. Whole language
practice is very politically oriented. We read on page 23:
Learning is a social process ... Although whole language educators accept
the importance of learning through individual interactions with the environment
(Piaget 1967), they lean more heavily on Vygotsky's ideas about the social nature
of learning (Vygotsky 1978).
Whole language takes seriously Vygotsky's notion of the Zone of Proximal
Development (Engstrom 1986) which entails stressing the importance of collaborations
(between students and teachers and between peers) through which students can
transcend their own individual limitations.
You might ask: Who is Vygotsky? Vygotsky (1896-1934) was a Soviet psychologist
who worked with Pavlov's colleagues at the State Institute of Experimental Psychology
in Moscow in the 1920s and '30s. James Wertsch, Vygotsky's biographer, writes:
[It] is important to note that Vygotsky was a staunch advocate of dialectical
and historical materialism. He was one of the creators of Marxist psychology
... People such as Vygotsky and his followers devoted every hour of their lives
to making certain that the new socialist state, the first grand experiment based
on Marxist-Leninist principles, would survive.
Vygotsky's colleague, Alexander Luria, wrote: "Vygotsky was ... the leading Marxist
theoretician among us ... in [his] hands, Marx's methods of analysis did serve a
vital role in shaping our course."
Apparently, these same methods of analysis are also serving to shape the course
of the whole-language agenda. The three professors, cited earlier, state on page
67:
The whole language theoretical premise underlying which topics are pursued
and how they are treated is: "All knowledge is socially constructed."
Therefore all knowing is political. In an effort to promote critical literacy
and thus to help children learn to read the world, not only the word (Shor &
[Marxist revolutionary] Freire 1987), teachers who work with theme cycles try
– no matter whether the topic is overtly "political" or not – to show how the
topic is related to other more general questions.
They try to demystify social institutions by helping children investigate
connections between surface facts and underlying social structures, between
lived experience and structural features of class, gender and race. They know
that not making connections is as political as making connections.
No further explanation needed. But what about phonics, you might ask? Here's
a view of phonics given in another book on whole language, "Evaluation: Whole Language,
Whole Child." We read on page 19:
The way you interpret what the child does will reflect what you understand
reading to be. For instance, if she reads the word feather for father,
a phonics-oriented teacher might be pleased because she's come close to sounding
the word out.
However, if you believe reading is a meaning-seeking process, you may be
concerned that she's overly dependent on phonics at the expense of meaning.
You'd be happier with a miscue such as daddy, even though it doesn't
look or sound anything like the word in the text. At least the meaning would
be intact.
My response to that kind of imbecilic pedagogy is that any child who looks at
the word "father" and says "daddy" can't read. It's as simple as that. But tell
that to a whole-language teacher. Meanwhile, we the taxpayers are paying for all
of it.
|